Abstract
Background: The video game industry has introduced a new form of monetization through microtransactions. A controversial example has been the so-called “loot boxes” (LBs) as virtual objects, which are randomized and bought with legal money. In recent years, LBs have come to connect 2 distinct problem behaviors, namely internet gaming disorder (IGD) and online gambling disorder (OGD). Many association studies have been conducted on the 3 constructs, but few have delved into the relationship of problematic use of LBs (PU-LB) with IGD and OGD.
Objective: This study aims to explore the mediating role of the PU-LB between IGD and OGD.
Methods: This cross-sectional and analytical study used incidental sampling in 24 Spanish schools. The final sample consisted of 542 participants (male: n=523, 96.5%; age: range 11‐30 y) who played video games, bought LBs, and had gambled online in the last 12 months. Participants then completed the Spanish versions of the Internet Gaming Disorder Scale–Short Form, Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire, and PU-LB scale.
Results: IGD scores were found to be significantly associated with both PU-LB (r=0.473, P<.001) and OGD (r=0.209, P<.001). Moreover, PU-LB was significantly associated with OGD (r=0.351, P<.001). The structural equation model results indicated that IGD had no significant direct effect on OGD (P=.903). However, the indirect effect of IGD on OGD through PU-LB was significant (P<.001). Therefore, PU-LB fully mediated the relationship between IGD and OGD. Furthermore, these results were found in the subsamples of both minors (<18 y) and young adults (≥18 y).
Conclusions: It is suggested that there is a mediation effect of problematic LB use between internet gambling and online gambling problems in both minors and young adults. This has potential practical implications by providing more evidence on how LBs have become a hinge feature between 2 clinically relevant and independent issues. In this regard, adequate industry self-regulation is needed, and effective legislation for the protection of minors is necessary.
doi:10.2196/57304
Keywords
Introduction
Video games are a form of interactive entertainment that has gained enormous popularity around the world. Video games can be seen even as a form of cultural expression because they reflect the creativity, values, beliefs, and experiences of both game developers and gamers alike [
].According to a report by DFC Intelligence, 40% of the global population plays video games, which represents an estimated 3.1 billion people playing video games as of 2022 [
]. In 2022, the global video game industry reached a market size of approximately US $246 billion [ ]. According to the Spanish Video Game Association [ ], in 2022, a total of 18.2 million people in Spain were recorded as gamers (there will be more than 48 million inhabitants in 2023), with 53% being male and 47% female. According to the report, Spaniards play video games for 7.42 hours per week (8% less than in the previous report), and turnover has risen to 2012 million euros (an increase of 12% compared with the previous year). Furthermore, the age profile of Spanish video game players is mainly young, with 79% between 6 and 11 years old, 84% between 11 and 14 years old, and 71% between 15 and 24 years old [ ]. In addition to the increase in video game sales, one of the factors behind this economic success lies in the incorporation of in-game purchases, which represent an increasingly large revenue stream for the industry [ , ]. In this regard, in 2023, a national study in Spain showed that 17.7% of adolescents between 14 and 18 years of age had gambled online. Of these, more than 50% claim to have gambled in the context of video games [ ].There is an ever-increasing number of video games that offer microtransactions (ie, the payment of a stipulated price for a specific, well-known skin or perk) [
]. Each video game features its own type of microtransactions, some of which are only of an aesthetic nature, while others may influence the dynamics of the game. Within the microtransactions, there is a special modality that in recent years has attracted the attention of researchers for its possible relation to random reward mechanisms: loot boxes (LBs), which are also called crates, cases, or chests. The acquisition of LBs involves the purchase of a virtual object (which can be acquired in various manifestations such as boxes, slot machines, chests, or in the form of animals), which is randomized and paid for with legal tender (this can be obtained from a prepaid card, a credit card payment or by prepurchasing currency from a game or environment to buy LBs) [ ]. The fact that this virtual object is the product of a random reward is what has made it similar to gambling, as both share a random reward mechanism [ - ]. Paradigmatic examples of LBs in video games may be the case analyzed by Lemmens [ ] on FIFA Ultimate Team and those analyzed by Xiao et al [ ], with some of the top downloaded games for Android, such as Game of Thrones: Conquest or Pokemon GO, including LBs.On the one hand, some primary studies have associated LB purchases with clinical problems with video games (usually assessed with questionnaires that follow the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition [DSM-5] criteria for internet gaming disorder [IGD]). Supporting evidence for this link has been found by authors in cross-sectional studies [
, , ], but there is hardly any longitudinal evidence [ ]. On the other hand, there is more evidence of a direct relationship between the purchase of LBs and problem gambling [ , , , , - ]. However, only a few studies have related the purchase of LBs to clinical problems of online gambling (following the DSM-5 [ ] and International Classification of Diseases, 11th Revision [ICD-11] criteria) [ ], and almost all these have been cross-sectional studies. One of them is the research with minors and adults by González-Cabrera et al [ ] carried out in Spain with a large sample of more than 6500 participants, where 3 out of every 10 participants purchased LBs in the last 12 months. Although limited, there is also evidence of the association of these problems over time in minors, with over 50% of LB purchasers still buying after 6 months [ ]. Studies have also already been conducted as adults linking LBs to gambling over time [ ].Overall, we still need to answer the possible hypotheses raised by Spicer et al [
] about LBs and gambling: either (1) users who gamble in other environments buy more LBs; (2) buyers of LBs are more likely to start gambling, through the “gateway effect”; or (3) there is a complex and dynamic relationship between both behaviors, where gambling is known to interact with other risky behaviors. In line with the latter suggestion, LBs have been related to gaming and gambling problems in minors and mostly adults [ , , , ]. Nevertheless, the possible mediating role of LBs between both clinical problems has been much less addressed. LBs have been a novel and relatively massive phenomenon over the past 5 years (approximately). This phenomenon has become the hinge that can bridge 2 clinical and nosological entities that appear separately in the diagnostic manuals [ ] and that have not been modified in the current revision of the DSM-5-TR [ ]. As such, a pioneering study was conducted with adults in which the mediation of microtransaction engagement between gaming and gambling was analyzed [ ]. This mediating construct was assessed using the Risky Loot-Box Index (RLI) [ ], which captures cognitive concern about LB use, impulsive use, and chasing losses, but it has limitations in terms of validity and reliability. In addition, this study did not use a clinical assessment tool for online gambling (and did not consider research with minors).Despite these limitations, the results were interesting as they did not achieve a complete mediation (ie, the direct relationship between video game problems and betting was also significant). Overall, the results indicate that participants with IGD were more likely to purchase microtransactions and to report more gambling-related problems. It is also possible that there have been significant changes in the consumption of LBs since the 2020 release (King et al [ ]), as this business model has become increasingly common and has grown in recent years [ ].This study, based on the study by King et al [
], included 2 clinical measures of online gaming and gambling problems and an instrument with adequate validity indicators of problematic LB use. In addition, a large sample of adults and minors (often less addressed in the literature) was included, with the latter requiring special safeguards regarding the LB phenomenon as covert gambling [ ]. The authors posit the hypothesis that there is no direct relationship between clinical problems with video games and online gambling, unless there is a problematic use of LBs (PU-LB) that mediates this relationship and therefore generates a significant indirect effect between the 2 clinical variables in the model. Thus, the aim was to perform a mediation of the PU-LB scale between IGD and online gambling disorder (OGD) in a sample of minors and young adults.Methods
Design and Recruitment
This study used a cross-sectional design. The sampling was incidental and was carried out in 24 nonuniversity educational centers in 8 Spanish regions (ie, Asturias, Cantabria, Castilla la Mancha, Castilla y Leon, Comunidad de Madrid, Comunidad Foral de Navarra, Comunidad Valenciana, and País Vasco). The educational stages covered ranged from compulsory secondary education (approximately between 11 and 16 y of age) to baccalaureate education (approximately between 16 and 18 y of age), with the addition of vocational training (where the ages range between 15 and 30 y old). This sample is part of a larger study on internet risks in adolescence. The final sample consisted of participants (both younger and older than 18 years) who had answered “yes” to the following 3 questions: have you played video games in the last 12 months, have you bought any video game LBs with money in the last 12 months, and have you gambled online in any type of game in the last 12 months?
Assessment Instruments
Participants were initially asked sociodemographic questions (gender, age, study center, and province).
IGD was assessed with the Spanish version of the Internet Gaming Disorder Scale–Short Form (IGD9-SF) [
- ]. This scale consists of 9 items based on the DSM-5 criteria for IGD (eg, “Have you deceived any of your family, therapists, or friends about the time you spend gaming?”) [ ]. The scale response options range from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). The total score ranges between 0 and 36, with greater scores suggesting higher symptom-severity of disordered gaming. In terms of internal reliability, the Cronbach α coefficient and ω coefficient in the present sample were 0.85 and 0.86, respectively.As for OGD, this construct was evaluated with the Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire (OGD-Q) [
]. This scale consists of 11 items that assess OGD in adolescence (eg, “Do you feel nervous, irritated, or angry when trying to reduce or stop gambling online?”). The scale response options range from 0 (never) to 4 (every day), with greater scores suggesting higher symptom-severity of disordered gambling. The total score ranges between 0 and 45. The scale Cronbach α coefficient and ω coefficient were 0.92 and 0.92, respectively.The PU-LB scale [
], consists of 18 items assessing the potentially problematic nature of engaging in LB purchasing behavior (eg, “Loot boxes have caused problems in my life (either social, economic, family, school, or work. etc” or “I usually buy loot boxes to feel better or happier”). The scale response options range from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), with total scores ranging from 0 to 90, where higher scores suggest a more PU-LB. In relation to its internal reliability, the Cronbach α coefficient and ω coefficient in the present sample were 0.92 and 0.87, respectively.Procedure
The survey was conducted online through the Survey Monkey platform on either a mobile device or computer. The participants were given access to and supervised by their teachers. The evaluation was carried out in educational centers (during the school timetable). The researchers previously trained the teachers in data collection. In addition, participant detection mechanisms were enabled, such as those suggested by Niessen et al [
], which included maximum response time “longstring” and “person-fit statistics.” The time needed to complete the questionnaires ranged between 5 and 15 minutes, depending on students’ age and reading comprehension.Ethical Considerations
The study was conducted with the authorization of all the participants in the investigation and with the consent of the school directors, students, and families. Students and families’ collaboration was voluntary, anonymous, and disinterested. The project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of International University of La Rioja (Spain) (PI007-2020 y PI001/2021), and the Juvenile Prosecutor’s Office was informed. The study received consent from all participants and school principals. Consent forms were sent to parents or guardians of participants younger than 18 years, and the purpose of the study was explained. About 0.8% of the participants did not want to respond to the questionnaire, while less than 1% of parents or guardians refused participation. Participants older than 18 years provided informed consent when completing the survey. Although there were no formal exclusion criteria, except for refusal to participate by parents or guardians for the overall sample, to be included in this study, participants had to answer affirmatively to a dichotomous question (yes or no) on whether they had played video games in the last 12 months, whether they had gambled online in the last 12 months, and whether they had bought LBs in the last 12 months. Only those who answered “yes” to each of these questions were assessed and included in the study.
Statistical Analysis
The SPSS (version 26; IBM Corp) program was used to (1) explore and screen all data through descriptive statistics; (2) test for reliability by Cronbach α, ω coefficient, and normality through skewness and kurtosis; and (3) explore the relationships between variables through bivariate correlations. The absolute values of skewness and kurtosis are normal when they are below ±3 for skewness and ±10 for kurtosis [
].MPLUS (version 8.0; Muthen & Muthen) [
] was used to test (1) the factor structure of the PU-LB through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and (2) the relationships between IGD, PU-LB, and OGD through structural equation modeling analysis. The Maximum Likelihood Robust Estimator was used, and the fit of the model was estimated with the most reliable fit indices [ ]: the Satorra-Bentler chi-square (S-Bχ2), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). A model was considered to adequately fit the data at values ≥.90 for the CFI and TLI, with values above .95 preferred, and values ≤.08 for the RMSEA and SRMR [ ]. The significance of mediational paths was tested by means of bias-corrected bootstrapping with 5000 samples.Results
Participants, Descriptive Statistics, Normality, and Reliability
The sample was composed of 542 participants (male: n=523, 96.5%) who played video games, purchased LBs, and gambled online in the last 12 months. The average age of the sample was 17.78 (SD 2.78) years (age: range 11‐30 y), out of which 47.2% (n=256) were minors (meanage 15.3, SD 1.71 y) and the remaining 56.3% (n=306) were young adults older than 18 years (meanage 19.6, SD 1.88 y). There were also 107 (19.7%) students in compulsory secondary education, 36 (6.6%) in baccalaureate education, and 399 (73.7%) in vocational training.
Descriptives, segregated by age groups (minors and adults), for all variables, including the means, SDs, and Pearson bivariate correlations between the variables of the study, are presented in
. Moreover, for the total sample (minors and adults), the descriptive statistics were as follows: for IGD, mean 8.216 (SD 0.301), skewness=1.023, and kurtosis=0.858; for PU-LB, mean 12.583 (SD 14.890), skewness=1.590, and kurtosis=2.794; and for OGD, mean 4.053 (SD 7.104), skewness=2.585, and kurtosis=7.385.IGD scores were found to be significantly associated with both PU-LB (r=0.473, P<.001) and OGD (r=0.209, P<.001). Additionally, PU-LB was significantly associated with OGD (r=0.351, P<.001).
Variables | Minors (n=256) | Young adults (n=306) | |||||||||
IGD | PU-LB | OGD | Questionnaire Score, mean (SD) | Skew | Kurt | Questionnaire Score, mean (SD) | Skew | Kurt | |||
IGD | — | 0.477 | 0.229 | 8.22 (7.07) | 1.02 | 1.23 | 8.2 (7.48) | 1.025 | 0.64 | ||
PU-LB | 0.484 | — | 0.354 | 15.33 (14.83) | 0.96 | 0.12 | 10.46 (14.6) | 2.203 | 6.02 | ||
OGD | 0.179 | 0.410 | — | 3.3 (6.09) | 3.19 | 12.18 | 4.62 (7.75) | 2.282 | 5.33 |
aSkew: skewness.
bKurt: kurtosis.
cNot applicable.
dAll correlations were significant at P<.001.
Structural Mediation Model
Before testing the structural mediation model, the CFA model of PU-LB (which had been validated through exploratory factor analysis but not through CFA) yielded some evidence for satisfactory fit (S-Bχ2125=300.207; RMSEA=.051, 90% CI .044-.058; CFI=.929; TLI=.913; SRMR=.054). The IGDS9-SF and OGD-Q had been previously validated and had shown good structural properties in previous studies [
, ].displays standardized path coefficients for the structural equation modeling with the total sample and subsamples of minors and young adults. The model for the overall sample had an adequate fit (S-Bχ2647=1156.821; RMSEA=.038, 90% CI .035-.042; CFI=.924; TLI=.918; SRMR=.055). All items loading onto latent variable were significant (P<.001) and ranged from .45 to .86. Moreover, model for the subsample of young adults (n=306) (S-Bχ2647=1057.634; RMSEA=.046, 90% CI .041-.050; CFI=.910; TLI=.908; SRMR=.064), and minors (n=236) (S-Bχ2647=980.688; RMSEA=.047, 90% CI .041-.052; CFI=.892; TLI=.882; SRMR=.065), showed inconsistent indexes for CFI and TLI, and while the RMSEA and SRMR values remain acceptable, the values for the CFI fail to meet the cut-off (≥.90). However, Raykov [ ] defended that CFI is a measure based on noncentrality and therefore, could be biased.
The results in the total sample indicated that IGD did not have a significant direct effect on OGD (β=.004, P=.903). However, the indirect effect of IGD on OGD by PU-LB was significant: (β=.223, 95% CI .131-.338, P<.001). Therefore, PU-LB fully mediated the relationship between IGD and OGD. In addition, these results were also found in the subsamples of minors (β=.327, 95% CI .177-.561, P<.001) and young adults (β=.187, 95% CI .072-.324, P<.001).
Discussion
The business model of many video game companies has been linked to LBs; this is worrying for many sectors of society, and this has generated wide interest in the academic and research context. Moreover, LBs appear to be a hinge that links 2 problem behaviors (IGD and OGD). In this regard, at an early stage in the study of LBs, King et al [
] conducted a mediation of microtransaction engagement between gaming and gambling in adults. There is evidence of an increase in the number of video games with LBs in recent years [ ] and the difficulty for laws restricting the use of LBs to be effective [ ]. The aim was of this study to perform a model that would allow us to analyze whether there would be a direct relationship between IGD and OGD in video game users who are LBs buyers and online gamblers in the last year or if, on the contrary, there would be an indirect effect thanks to the mediation of the PU-LB. The results suggest that because of the significant indirect effect (and the absence of direct effect), there is a total mediation effect. This reinforces the idea that the PU-LB may be a problem connecting 2 different problem behaviors (IGD and OGD) and that LBs, as a random reward mechanism, can be associated with gambling as a gateway or as a further gambling mechanism [ ]. Thus, LBs seem to have spurred and connected 2 very pernicious issues to each other, adding a layer of complexity to the problem. Furthermore, this phenomenon is especially worrisome because minors are involved in these mechanisms (the model adjusts for the total sample and for minors and adults). From these data, important theoretical implications are derived. On the one hand, problems with gaming do not have a direct effect on gambling, and on the other hand, PU-LB generates a full mediation, justifying that hinge role. It should be noted that the entire study sample consisted of buyers of LBs, so it is not only necessary to buy them but also to present a problem with them. This is also related to the emphasis of the ICD-11 [ ] on the consequences that behavioral disorders should have. With the above, it is clear that industry mechanisms do not seem to be sufficient to regulate this process [ ], nor are the governmental measures taken in some countries to curb the problem [ ].The study by King et al [
], as well as other studies, used the RLI to assess the risk of LB use [ , , ]. In general, the RLI shows deficiencies in its psychometric validation process, as well as drawbacks for not covering other key aspects related to the problems that may arise due to the behavior of purchasing LBs (eg, impulsivity to buy more LBs, personal consequences, salience of play time or guilt, among others). For this reason, we used the PU-LB [ ], which presents adequate validity and reliability indicators; its content includes validity indicators of general problems about LBs and specific indicators regarding the association among LBs, gaming, and gambling (mood regulation through the purchase or opening of LBs, postponing activities to get LBs, feeling the urge to buy them, thinking about the purchase activity or feeling bad about the time or money invested, etc). Furthermore, contributing to the pioneering work of King et al [ ] is the use of clinical questionnaires (IGDS9-SF and OGD-Q) based on the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-5 [ ] and ICD-11 [ ] (including minors and adults). In this sense, there are no data to compare with, as this study has a singular focus.This study has some relevant limitations. First, only self-report measures were used, which may generate response bias and social desirability bias. Second, there may be a retrospective recall bias, as participants were asked to think back to what they did in the last 12 months. Third, although the sample of participants was large and geographically dispersed, the sampling was not random, so it is not representative of the Spanish context. Fourth, there is an overrepresentation of male participants in the study, which is a common issue in many studies, since consumers of video games, gambling and LBs are mostly male. However, these data also indicate that boys require special attention regarding the LB problem (at least in the Spanish context). Fifth, the OGD-Q questionnaire is validated in Spanish adolescents (up to the age of 19 years) and not in adults. Although the reliability indicators are adequate, this may be a limitation of the study. Sixth, all parameters, apart from the OGD variable in the group of minors, exhibited skewness and kurtosis values indicative of a normal distribution. However, it is emphasized that the lack of normality does not pose a methodological obstacle, as a robust approach was used to effectively address the presence of nonnormal distributions in the statistical analysis. Finally, the fit indices of the model for minors are slightly below the thresholds considered good (particularly the CFI and TLI). However, this may be due to the nature of the constructs and the fact that online gambling is an illegal activity for minors; therefore, the data related to the OGD may have affected the model in general.
Given these potential limitations, future research should include longitudinal designs that take into account the variables used in this study and answer the questions posed by Spicer et al [
] on the “gateway effect” of LBs or the relationship between who opens and who purchases LBs, as opening is likely to focus more on gaming problems and purchasing is likely to focus more on gambling. As such, exploring the independent and additive effect of both roles may be an area for future research. However, this study has been able to address questions also raised by these authors in relation to the relationship between gaming and gambling problems. This study has potential practical implications by providing more evidence on how LBs have become a hinge feature between 2 clinically relevant and independent issues. While there has been very strict legislation on LBs in countries such as Belgium [ ], it has not been effective because it has not been properly enforced. Despite this situation, there is still a need for politicians to properly regulate the LB framework and prevent minors from purchasing LBs. The key aspect is to enforce the law and provide resources to do so. In the Spanish context, there has been a draft bill in place since 2022 to regulate random reward mechanisms associated with interactive software products [ ]. This draft bill has basic limitations such as a remarkably restrictive definition of a LB (article 3, point C), which, if approved, will be clearly insufficient. This is why, in the Spanish case, it is necessary to improve legislative efforts and add other actions. In this sense, it could help to make it a requirement for the video game industry to include information on LBs (within the framework of the Pan-European Gaming Information [PEGI] system), but this must be clear, specific, and comprehensible [ ]. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, there is also a need for psychoeducational actions aimed primarily at preventing the purchase of LBs by minors. These actions should be evidence-based. Education of minors and their families is key and is the future course of action.In conclusion, this study suggests that there is a mediation effect of problematic LB use between IGD and OGD in both minors and young adults.
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by Universidad Internacional de La Rioja (UNIR Research Plan [2022-2024]), Ministerio de Consumo (Dirección General de Ordenación del Juego-SUBV23/00022), and ITEI-UNIR (B23-006 & B24-001).
Data Availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Conflicts of Interest
JGC has received funding from the State Programme for R&D&I Oriented to the Challenges of Society (Programa Estatal de I+D+I Orientada a los retos de la Sociedad) and is currently receiving funding for research projects at the Universidad Internacional de La Rioja (Spain) and the Ministry of Consumer (Ministerio de Consumo) in Spain (in particular, for the study of loot boxes). He has also provided consultancy services for the company TICandBot (Logroño, Spain). All this funding did not involve any real conflict in the conduct of this research. The authors have no further competing interests to disclose.
References
- Muriel D, Crawford G. Video Games as Culture: Considering the Role and Importance of Video Games in Contemporary Society. Routledge; 2018.
- Crece el público gamer: un estudio revela sus ganancias y tendencias esenciales para este año. Forbes. 2022. URL: https://www.forbesargentina.com/innovacion/crece-publico-gamer-estudio-revela-sus-ganancias tendencias-esenciales-ano-n16215 [Accessed 2022-05-25]
- Evolución del valor de mercado de la industria del videojuego en el mundo entre 2022 y 2030. Videogame indusrty market evolution in the world between 2022 and 2030. Statista. URL: https://es.statista.com/estadisticas/598622/valor-de-mercado-del-videojuego-en-el-mundo/#:~:text=En%202022%2C%20la%20industria%20del,los%20680.000%20millones%20en%202030 [Accessed 2024-09-15]
- La industria del videojuego en España en 2022. The video game industry in Spain in 2022. Asociación Española de Videojuegos. 2024. URL: https://www.aevi.org.es/web/wp-content/uploads/2024/01/AEVI_INFOGRAPHIC-22-ENGLISH.pdf [Accessed 2020-01-18]
- Kristiansen S, Severin MC. Loot box engagement and problem gambling among adolescent gamers: findings from a national survey. Addict Behav. Apr 2020;103:106254. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Li W, Mills D, Nower L. The relationship of loot box purchases to problem video gaming and problem gambling. Addict Behav. Oct 2019;97:27-34. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Observatorio Español de las Drogas y las Adicciones. Informe sobre: Adicciones comportamentales y otros trastornos adictivos 2023. Portal Plan Nacional sobre Drogas. 2023. URL: https://pnsd.sanidad.gob.es/profesionales/sistemasInformacion/sistemaInformacion/pdf/2023_Informe_Trastornos_Comportamentales.pdf [Accessed 2024-09-13]
- King DL, Delfabbro PH. Loot box limit-setting is not sufficient on its own to prevent players from overspending: a reply to Drummond, Sauer & Hall. Addiction. Jul 2019;114(7):1324-1325. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Montiel I, Basterra-González A, Machimbarrena JM, Ortega-Barón J, González-Cabrera J. Loot box engagement: a scoping review of primary studies on prevalence and association with problematic gaming and gambling. PLoS One. 2022;17(1):e0263177. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Drummond A, Sauer JD, Hall LC, Zendle D, Loudon MR. Author correction: why loot boxes could be regulated as gambling. Nat Hum Behav. Aug 11, 2020;5(6):802-802. [CrossRef]
- Zendle D, Cairns P. Video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: results of a large-scale survey. PLoS One. 2018;13(11):e0206767. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lemmens JS. Play or pay to win: loot boxes and gaming disorder in FIFA Ultimate Team. Tele Inform Rep. Dec 2022;8:100023. [CrossRef]
- Xiao LY, Henderson LL, Newall PWS. Loot boxes are more prevalent in United Kingdom video games than previously considered: updating Zendle et al. (2020). Addiction. Sep 2022;117(9):2553-2555. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Evren C, Evren B, Dalbudak E, Topcu M, Kutlu N. The relationship of loot box engagement to gender, severity of disordered gaming, using MMORPGs, and motives for online gaming. PBS. 2021;11(1):25. [CrossRef]
- Zendle D, Meyer R, Cairns P, Waters S, Ballou N. The prevalence of loot boxes in mobile and desktop games. Addiction. Sep 2020;115(9):1768-1772. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- González-Cabrera J, Basterra-González A, Ortega-Barón J, et al. Loot box purchases and their relationship with internet gaming disorder and online gambling disorder in adolescents: a prospective study. Comput Human Behav. Jun 2023;143:107685. [CrossRef]
- Brooks GA, Clark L. Associations between loot box use, problematic gaming and gambling, and gambling-related cognitions. Addict Behav. Sep 2019;96:26-34. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Close J, Spicer SG, Nicklin LL, Uther M, Lloyd J, Lloyd H. Secondary analysis of loot box data: are high-spending “whales” wealthy gamers or problem gamblers? Addict Behav. Jun 2021;117:106851. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Garea SS, Drummond A, Sauer JD, Hall LC, Williams MN. Meta-analysis of the relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming and loot box spending. Int Gambl Stud. Sep 2, 2021;21(3):460-479. [CrossRef]
- Macey J, Hamari J. eSports, skins and loot boxes: participants, practices and problematic behaviour associated with emergent forms of gambling. New Media Soc. Jan 2019;21(1):20-41. [CrossRef]
- von Meduna M, Steinmetz F, Ante L, Reynolds J, Fiedler I. Loot boxes are gambling-like elements in video games with harmful potential: results from a large-scale population survey. Technol Soc. Nov 2020;63:101395. [CrossRef]
- Wardle H, Zendle D. Loot boxes, gambling, and problem gambling among young people: results from a cross-sectional online survey. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. Apr 2021;24(4):267-274. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zendle D, Meyer R, Over H. Adolescents and loot boxes: links with problem gambling and motivations for purchase. R Soc Open Sci. Jun 2019;6(6):190049. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zendle D, Cairns P. Correction: video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: results of a large-scale survey. PLoS One. 2019;14(3):e0214167. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
- World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (11th Revision). World Health Organization; 2018.
- González-Cabrera J, Basterra-González A, Montiel I, Calvete E, Pontes HM, Machimbarrena JM. Loot boxes in Spanish adolescents and young adults: relationship with internet gaming disorder and online gambling disorder. Comput Human Behav. Jan 2022;126:107012. [CrossRef]
- Brooks GA, Clark L. The gamblers of the future? migration from loot boxes to gambling in a longitudinal study of young adults. Comput Human Behav. Apr 2023;141:107605. [CrossRef]
- Spicer SG, Nicklin LL, Uther M, Lloyd J, Lloyd H, Close J. Loot boxes, problem gambling and problem video gaming: a systematic review and meta-synthesis. New Media Soc. Apr 2022;24(4):1001-1022. [CrossRef]
- Gibson E, Griffiths MD, Calado F, Harris A. The relationship between videogame micro-transactions and problem gaming and gambling: a systematic review. Comput Human Behav. Jun 2022;131:107219. [CrossRef]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-5-TR). American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2022. [CrossRef] ISBN: 978-0-89042-575-6
- King A, Wong-Padoongpatt G, Barrita A, Phung DT, Tong T. Risk factors of problem gaming and gambling in US emerging adult non-students: the role of loot boxes, microtransactions, and risk-taking. Issues Ment Health Nurs. Dec 2020;41(12):1063-1075. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Beranuy M, Machimbarrena JM, Vega-Osés MA, et al. Spanish validation of the Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form (IGDS9-SF): prevalence and relationship with online gambling and quality of life. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Feb 28, 2020;17(5):1562. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Pontes HM, Király O, Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD. The conceptualisation and measurement of DSM-5 internet gaming disorder: the development of the IGD-20 Test. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110137. [CrossRef]
- Pontes HM, Griffiths MD. Measuring DSM-5 internet gaming disorder: development and validation of a short psychometric scale. Comput Human Behav. Apr 2015;45:137-143. [CrossRef]
- González-Cabrera J, Machimbarrena JM, Beranuy M, Pérez-Rodríguez P, Fernández-González L, Calvete E. Design and measurement properties of the Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire (OGD-Q) in Spanish adolescents. J Clin Med. Jan 2, 2020;9(1):120. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Niessen ASM, Meijer RR, Tendeiro JN. Detecting careless respondents in web-based questionnaires: which method to use? J Res Pers. Aug 2016;63:1-11. [CrossRef]
- Bentler PM, Bonett DG. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol Bull. 1980;88(3):588-606. [CrossRef]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 8th ed. Muthén & Muthén; 1998.
- Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. Jan 1999;6(1):1-55. [CrossRef]
- Raykov T. Bias-corrected estimation of noncentrality parameters of covariance structure models. Struct Equ Modeling. Jan 2005;12(1):120-129. [CrossRef]
- Xiao LY. Breaking ban: Belgium’s ineffective gambling law regulation of video game loot boxes. Psychology (Irvine). Jan 17, 2023;9(1):57641. [CrossRef]
- Garrett EP, Drummond A, Lowe-Calverley E, Sauer JD. Current loot box warnings are ineffective for informing consumers. Comput Human Behav. Feb 2023;139:107534. [CrossRef]
- Xiao LY. Beneath the label: unsatisfactory compliance with ESRB, PEGI and IARC industry self-regulation requiring loot box presence warning labels by video game companies. R Soc Open Sci. Mar 2023;10(3):230270. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics. World Health Organization. 2018. URL: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en [Accessed 2020-01-18]
- Ministerio de Consumo. Anteproyecto de Ley por el que se regulan los mecanismos aleatorios de recompensa asociados a productos de software interactivo de ocio. Draft bill regulating random reward mechanisms associated with interactive leisure software products. Ministerio de Derechos Sociales, Consumo y Agenda 2030. 2022. URL: https://www.consumo.gob.es/sites/consumo.gob.es/files/BORRADOR%20APL%20Y%20MAIN%20MECANISMOS%20ALEATORIOS%20RECOMPENSA%20010722.pdf [Accessed 2024-09-05]
Abbreviations
CFA: confirmatory factor analysis |
CFI: comparative fit index |
DSM-5: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition |
ICD-11: International Classification of Diseases, 11th Revision |
IGD: internet gaming disorder |
IGD9-SF: Internet Gaming Disorder Scale–Short Form |
LB: loot box |
OGD: online gambling disorder |
OGD-Q: Online Gambling Disorder Questionnaire |
PEGI: Pan-European Gaming Information |
PU-LB: problematic use of loot boxes |
RLI: Risky Loot-Box Index |
RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation |
S-Bχ2: Satorra-Bentler chi-square |
SRMR: standardized root mean square residual |
TLI: Tucker-Lewis index |
Edited by Amaryllis Mavragani; submitted 12.02.24; peer-reviewed by Anu Sirola, Leon Xiao, Lucia Monacis; final revised version received 17.06.24; accepted 17.06.24; published 20.09.24.
Copyright© Joaquín González-Cabrera, Vanessa Caba-Machado, Adoración Díaz-López, Susana Jiménez-Murcia, Gemma Mestre-Bach, Juan M Machimbarrena. Originally published in JMIR Serious Games (https://games.jmir.org), 20.9.2024.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Serious Games, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://games.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.